In Chapter 11, we extended the logic of the experiment that has two levels of one independent variable to experiments that have more that two levels of one independent variable, as in the example below:
Level of Caffeine | 0 mg | 25 mg | 50 mg | 75mg | 100 mg |
That study would not be a factorial study because only one factor is being manipulated. Because only one factor is being manipulated, it cannot be used to make conclusions about
In Chapter 12, we extend the logic of the experiment that uses one independent variable to experiments that use
two or more independent variables. The main advantages of studying more than two independent variables at a time are
To illustrate, consider the 2 X 2 factorial experiment described in the table below:
No Caffeine | Caffeine | |
---|---|---|
No Noise | Group 1 | Group 2 |
Noise | Group 3 | Group 4 |
If, on the other hand, there is no interaction, then you can talk about the effects of noise and caffeine separately. You do not have to qualify your statements about the effects of noise by saying things like "However, the effect of noise is qualified by a noise by caffeine interaction. The effect of noise is different in the no caffeine condition than in the caffeine condition."
To appreciate the difference between main effects and interactions, consider the hypothetical data from the experiment below:
Experiment A
No Caffeine | Caffeine | |
---|---|---|
No Noise | 2 | 4 |
Noise | 5 | 7 |
In this experiment, noise has an effect: The noise groups score 3 points higher, on average, than the no-noise groups. Caffeine also has an effect: The caffeine groups score, on average, 2 points higher than the no-caffeine groups. However, there is no interaction because the combination of caffeine and noise produces the same effect as we would expect from their individual effects. For example, knowing that the caffeine effect is, on the average 2, how would you complete the table below? (Don't look at the Experiment A table until you are finished.)
No Caffeine | Caffeine | |
---|---|---|
No Noise | 2 | |
Noise | 5 |
Similarly, knowing that the noise effect is 3, on the average, how would you complete the table below? (don't look at the other tables until you are finished.)
No Caffeine | Caffeine | |
---|---|---|
No Noise | 2 | 4 |
Noise |
So, for Experiment A, the average effects of your variables tells the whole
story.
Contrast that with Experiment B, where, as before, the average effect of caffeine
is 2 and the average effect of noise is 3, but, this time, there is an interaction.
No Caffeine | Caffeine | |
---|---|---|
No Noise | 2 | 3 |
Noise | 4 | 7 |
Because there is an interaction in Experiment B, the average effects don't tell the whole story. As you can see, caffeine has more of an effect in the noise groups (7-4 =3) than it does for the no-noise groups (3-2 =1). So, if you tried to predict caffeine's effects knowing only its average effect, you would make mistakes. For example, you would probably fill in this table as you did before
No Caffeine | Caffeine | |
---|---|---|
No Noise | 2 | |
Noise | 4 |
like so:
No Caffeine | Caffeine | |
---|---|---|
No Noise | 2 | 4 |
Noise | 4 | 6 |
which is not correct.
To get a general idea of what interactions represent, see Table 12-1 and 12-2 (pp. 475-477).
After explaining interactions, we made two important distinctions.
First, we distinguished between ordinal and disordinal (cross-over) interactions. We pointed out that you can easily tell which type of interaction you have by graphing it. More importantly, we showed how ordinal interactions--rather than meaning that the combination of two variables has a smaller (or bigger) psychological effect than would be expected by looking at the variables' individual effects--could be due to having ordinal scale data (your scores not accurately describing how much more of a quantity participants had). Cross-over interactions, on the other hand, can't be due to having ordinal scale data. Thus, we can usually be more confident of that a cross-over interaction indicates that combining your factors has an effect that is different from the sum of their individual effects. .
Second, we distinguished between "true " ( "strong") independent variables that we can randomly assign (e.g., level of caffeine) and "weak " independent variables that we can't randomly assign (e.g., participant variables, such as gender, personality type, etc.) . We stressed that you can't make causal statements about "weak " independent variables (variables that we do not manipulate).
Finally, you learned that you can add a second variable to a simple experiment to make it a 2 X 2 design. For example, to see whether the main effect of the variable studied in the simple experiment