Box 1: Sensory
Memory (abbreviated SM)--
a very brief, exact copy (a trace) of what you just sensed
Imagine that you are looking at the television, but not paying attention to the
program because you are talking to a friend. Suddenly, your friend points to the
television set. Thanks to your sensory memory (really memories), you can hit a rewind
button in your mind that allows you to replay the last little bit of the program.
That replay involves two sensory memories.
- When you replay the program's audio, you are using your echoic memory: the sensory memory for
hearing that allows you to, in a sense, hear an
echo of what just happened.
- When you replay the image that was on the television screen, you are using your
visual sensory memory (typically called
iconic memory).
Let's explore these 2 memories in more depth, starting with echoic memory.
Your echoic memory allows you to replay the last 3-5 seconds of what
your ears just sensed.
Having
access to instant replay of the last 3-5 seconds of what your ears just
sensed allows you to
- Know whether a sound is coming from your right or your left because
your echoic memory helps you figure out whether the sound hit your left ear or right ear first.
You are so good at figuring out where sounds come from that
you can learn to "see" with your ears--a little like a bat does!
- Effortlessly keep all the sounds of an entire word together.
(If you don't believe me about how effortlessly you do this or you just want to see what a pain it is to understand spoken words without echoic
memory, click the first link below--but remember, I did warn you! Then, to appreciate how effortless understanding
speech is with echoic memory, click the second link below. After that, you will
probably want to thank your
echoic memory.)
- When speech is artificially slowed down so much that you can't use echoic memory
to keep the sounds together, you can still understand words, but you will
probably find the effort to understand the words both exhausting and unpleasant (demo#1).
- With speech presented at a normal rate, you can use echoic memory, so keeping the sounds
together is much easier. (demo #2)
- Replay words or phrases that you initially misheard (e.g., you may have misheard
"plan it" as "planet" or euthanasia" as "youth in Asia").
- Act like you were listening when you weren't. For example, suppose a teacher believes you were not listening. Although you weren't listening, you say you
were. The teacher challenges you by asking, "If you were listening, then, what
did I say?" In response, you may have used your echoic memory to repeat the last few words of
what the teacher said even though you weren't listening. Two practical implications:
- The next time someone says "what?" or "huh?" right after you said something,
rather than repeating yourself, you could simply make them use their echoic memory to
hear an "echo" of what you just said.
- If your "listener" keeps repeating the last few words of your sentences, do not assume they are really listening to what you are saying.
Iconic ("eye"-conic) memory captures what your eyes captured. So, an iconic
memory is, in a sense, a
picture drawn in
rapidly disappearing ink. See a very short (less than a
second) demonstration of how quickly information decays from iconic memory Because pictures in iconic memory are copies of what you
just saw, are "drawn" automatically, and usually
fade in less than
half a second, it wasn't until
- 1740 when a scientist discovered iconic memory (the
first 20 seconds of this youTube video resembles a re-enactment of that
discovery). Iconic memory (sometimes called "persistence of vision") is usually easy to overlook because it is (1)
an exact copy of what you saw, (2) automatic, and (3) fleeting.
- 1960 when a scientist discovered that sensory memory could hold more than a few items.
Click on one of the links below to find out more about how that
scientist--George Sperling--figured out that sensory memory was large but short.
- Probably just now when you realized that if you watch a 2-hour (non-digital) movie
in a movie theater,
you are looking at a blank, dark screen for 1 hour (explanation).
A note about iconic memory's role in seeing movies:
If slides are projected on a screen at a rate of 12 slides or more per
second, the slide on the screen stays in your iconic memory during the gap
between slides.
Because you are "seeing" the memory of the previous slide instead of the gap between the slides, you see "moving pictures"
(i.e., "movies" or "motion pictures").
If,
on the other hand, slides
are projected at a rate slower than 12 a second, the previous image is gone from iconic memory
before you see the next image. Because you
are not seeing a memory of the previous slide and are instead seeing
the gaps between each slide, you see a slide show rather than a movie.
Iconic memory is not just valuable when watching movies. To get a rough idea of how confusing the world
be if you didn't have both iconic memory and visual short-term memory, watch this
48-second animation/simulation.
Iconic memory does not seem to be related to what we consider intelligence:
- Some people have relatively long iconic memories (these people are called "eidetikers"). Their "photographic memory" can hold an
image for seconds or even minutes. However, their superior iconic memory seems to be a worthless
superpower: Most people who have it
tend to be young children--and those children usually outgrow it. In addition,
college students who are eidetikers seem to get, if anything, lower grades than other
college students.
-
Chimps do not do well on human tests of intelligence. However, young chimps have
better iconic memories than we do. See chimps demonstrate their amazing iconic memory (Although many
press reports credited the chimps' impressive performances to "short-term memory," the chimps' impressive performances were probably due to having impressively
long iconic memories. That is, young chimps, like some young human
children, seem to be eidetikers.)
Key Characteristics of Sensory Memory
|
Types |
At least 2: Your sensory memory for vision--iconic memory--and your sensory memory for hearing--echoic memory. You may also have
ones for some of your other senses. |
Encoding |
Automatic: To encode information, you just need to sense the
information. |
Storage Size |
Enormous (maybe as much as 14 trillion bits!) |
Storage Duration |
Short--Less than half a second for iconic memory; about 3- 5 seconds
for echoic memory |
Silly summary |
"I have a good memory, but it's short" would be silly to say about
your long-term memory, but is probably accurate to say about your iconic
memory. |
Drag and drop matching game to test your knowledge of
sensory memory
(Play several rounds of this game; each round gets a little harder. Try to
rack up at least 9 points.)
Getting information from Sensory Memory to Short Term Memory
Short Term Memory
Back to the 3 Box Model
Back to Memory Menu
Back to Lecture Menu
Copyright 2020-2024 Mark L. Mitchell